[外语类试卷]大学英语六级改革适用(长篇阅读)模拟试卷1及答案与解析.doc
《[外语类试卷]大学英语六级改革适用(长篇阅读)模拟试卷1及答案与解析.doc》由会员分享,可在线阅读,更多相关《[外语类试卷]大学英语六级改革适用(长篇阅读)模拟试卷1及答案与解析.doc(20页珍藏版)》请在麦多课文档分享上搜索。
1、大学英语六级改革适用(长篇阅读)模拟试卷 1及答案与解析 Section B 0 Now Hiring: Green-Collar Workers AIf youre looking for a good, safe job, it makes sense these days to go green. The Obama Administration, which is directing billions of dollars to the environmental sector, projects that by 2016, green jobs will grow by 52% fr
2、om 2,000 levels. Green jobs, which do not necessarily require a science degree, range from environmental scientist to nonprofit executive for a wildlife or pro-transit advocacy group. BWhen Alden Zeitz started the Wind Energy Program at Iowa Lakes Community College five years ago, 15 students enroll
3、ed. In 2009, 102 students enrolled in the two-year training program for wind turbine technicians, including some students who abandoned another career for the economic promise of green technology. The wind energy industry hasnt been immune to the recession, but students are counting on the federal g
4、overnments injection of $80 billion in clean energy projects to change that. Stimulus for Green Energy CThe American Recovery while Y is struggling to help, X is switched for Z and Y fails to notice. Researchers are still pondering the full implications, but it does show how little information we us
5、e in daily life, and undermines the idea that we know what is going on around us. EWhen we set out, we aimed to weigh in on the enduring, complicated debate about self-knowledge and intentionality. For all the intimate familiarity we feel we have with decision-making, it is very difficult to know ab
6、out it from the “inside“: one of the great barriers for scientific research is the nature of subjectivity. FAs anyone who has ever been in a verbal disagreement can prove, people tend to give elaborate justifications for their decisions, which we have every reason to believe are nothing more than ra
7、tionalisations(文过饰非 )after the event. To prove such people wrong, though, or even provide enough evidence to change their mind, is an entirely different matter: who are you to say what my reasons are? GBut with choice blindness we drive a large wedge between intentions and actions in the mind. As ou
8、r participants give us verbal explanations about choices they never made, we can show them beyond doubt and prove it that what they say cannot be true. So our experiments offer a unique window into confabulation(虚构 )(the story-telling we do to justify things after the fact)that is otherwise very dif
9、ficult to come by. We can compare everyday explanations with those under lab conditions, looking for such things as the amount of detail in descriptions, how coherent the narrative is, the emotional tone, or even the timing or flow of the speech. Then we can create a theoretical framework to analyse
10、 any kind of exchange. HThis framework could provide a clinical use for choice blindness: for example, two of our ongoing studies examine how malingering(装病 )might develop into true symptoms, and how confabulation might play a role in obsessive-compulsive disorder(强迫症 ). IImportantly, the effects of
11、 choice blindness go beyond snap judgments. Depending on what our volunteers say in response to the mismatched outcomes of choices(whether they give short or long explanations, give numerical rating or labelling, and so on)we found this interaction could change their future preferences to the extent
12、 that they come to prefer the previously rejected alternative. This gives us a rare glimpse into the complicated dynamics of self-feedback(“I chose this, I publicly said so, therefore I must like it“), which we suspect lies behind the formation of many everyday preferences. JWe also want to explore
13、the boundaries of choice blindness. Of course, it will be limited by choices we know to be of great importance in everyday life. Which bride or bridegroom would fail to notice if someone switched their partner at the altar through amazing sleight of hand(巧妙的手段 )? Yet there is ample territory between
14、 the absurd idea of spouse-swapping, and the results of our early face experiments. KFor example, in one recent study we invited supermarket customers to choose between two paired varieties of jam and tea. In order to switch each participants choice without them noticing, we created two sets of “mag
15、ical“ jars, with lids at both ends and a divider inside. The jars looked normal, but were designed to hold one variety of jam or tea at each end, and could easily be flipped over. LImmediately after the participants chose, we asked them to taste their choice again and tell us verbally why they made
16、that choice. Before they did, we turned over the sample containers, so the tasters were given the opposite of what they had intended in their selection. Strikingly, people detected no more than a third of all these trick trials. Even when we switched such remarkably different flavors as spicy cinnam
17、on and apple for bitter grapefruit jam, the participants spotted less than half of all switches. MWe have also documented this kind of effect when we simulate online shopping for consumer products such as laptops or cellphones, and even apartments. Our latest tests are exploring moral and political
18、decisions, a domain where reflection and deliberation are supposed to play a central role, but which we believe is perfectly suited to investigating using choice blindness. NThroughout our experiments, as well as registering whether our volunteers noticed that they had been presented with the altern
19、ative they did not choose, we also quizzed them about their beliefs about their decision processes. How did they think they would feel if they had been exposed to a study like ours? Did they think they would have noticed the switches? Consistently, between 80 and 90 per cent of people said that they
20、 believed they would have noticed something was wrong. OImagine their surprise, even disbelief, when we told them about the nature of the experiments. In everyday decisionmaking we do see ourselves as knowing a lot about our selves, but like the wine buff or art critic, we often overstate what we kn
21、ow. The good news is that this form of decision snobbery should not be too difficult to treat. Indeed, after reading this article you might already be cured. 11 People have a tendency to try to give an acceptable explanation for the decision they make. 12 In their latest tests researchers are invest
22、igating peoples decisions in the fields of morals and politics with choice blindness. 13 The result of the face choosing experiments showed that most participants didnt realize that their choices had been switched. 14 Instead of playing tricks with things offered to participants, the researchers sec
23、retly changed the things they chose. 15 From the quiz researchers find that most people are quite confident about their feelings in the decision processes. 16 Change blindness refers to the phenomenon that many people fail to notice the big change around them. 17 The author says that some experts so
24、metimes fail to do well as claimed. 18 The volunteers were surprised at the fact that in everyday decision-making, peoples beliefs are often overstated. 19 Researchers suspect that the mechanism of self-feedback is the drive for many everyday preferences. 20 The boundary of choice blindness is not l
- 1.请仔细阅读文档,确保文档完整性,对于不预览、不比对内容而直接下载带来的问题本站不予受理。
- 2.下载的文档,不会出现我们的网址水印。
- 3、该文档所得收入(下载+内容+预览)归上传者、原创作者;如果您是本文档原作者,请点此认领!既往收益都归您。
下载文档到电脑,查找使用更方便
2000 积分 0人已下载
下载 | 加入VIP,交流精品资源 |
- 配套讲稿:
如PPT文件的首页显示word图标,表示该PPT已包含配套word讲稿。双击word图标可打开word文档。
- 特殊限制:
部分文档作品中含有的国旗、国徽等图片,仅作为作品整体效果示例展示,禁止商用。设计者仅对作品中独创性部分享有著作权。
- 关 键 词:
- 外语类 试卷 大学 英语六级 改革 适用 长篇 阅读 模拟 答案 解析 DOC
