[外语类试卷]大学英语六级改革适用(阅读)模拟试卷126及答案与解析.doc
《[外语类试卷]大学英语六级改革适用(阅读)模拟试卷126及答案与解析.doc》由会员分享,可在线阅读,更多相关《[外语类试卷]大学英语六级改革适用(阅读)模拟试卷126及答案与解析.doc(17页珍藏版)》请在麦多课文档分享上搜索。
1、大学英语六级改革适用(阅读)模拟试卷 126及答案与解析 Section A 0 According to a scientific study published in April, 2007, birds have shown they can plan for a future state of mind. Hoarding【 C1】 _for future use is not unique to humans. Birds, squirrels and monkeys do it. But the ability to think not just about tomorrow, b
2、ut to realize how tomorrows feelings might differ from todays, was thought to be the【 C2】 _of people. This week researchers demonstrated that Western scrub-jays, a type of crow, can do it, too. The researchers, led by Nicky Clayton of the University of Cambridge, wanted to test an idea proposed by W
3、olfgang Kohler, Norbert Bischof and Doris Bischof Kohler, three German psychologists. The Bischof Kohler hypothesis says that only humans can mentally separate themselves from what they are experiencing to【 C3】 _how they might feel about future events. To test whether this is so, Dr. Clayton and her
4、 colleagues sought to tease apart scrub-jays【 C4】 _desires from their planning for future needs. They let the birds eat as much of one food as they wanted, exploiting a condition called specific satiety(饱足 )once the birds are full of one food, they show strong【 C5】 _for something different. They the
5、n offered the birds that same food or a second one to store for later.【 C6】 _the scrub-jays behaved as predicted, choosing to stow away the second food, which they had not just eaten. But minutes before allowing the birds to【 C7】_their storage, the researchers fed the birds to satiety with that seco
6、nd food the one they had already stored. The birds changed their hoarding preferences on the very next trial. Even though they had just had their fill of the first food, they still hoard it, presumably because they thought it would be their preferred choice later. The results are published in this w
7、eeks Current Biology. The finding matters because the birds seem to plan ahead for what they will want later, even though their choice conflicts with what they want now. It could prompt a【 C8】_of how animals perceive the world around them. Without the benefit of【 C9】_subjects who can explain their t
8、hinking? However, Dr. Clayton and her colleagues will have to develop ever more cunning experiments to【 C10】 _complex mental processes from simple behavior. A)experimental I)contend B)preference J)momentary C)incidentally K)infer D)conceive L)initially E)reassessment M)homogeneous F)explicit N)prese
9、rve G)recover O)predecessor H)provisions 1 【 C1】 2 【 C2】 3 【 C3】 4 【 C4】 5 【 C5】 6 【 C6】 7 【 C7】 8 【 C8】 9 【 C9】 10 【 C10】 Section B 10 Does the Internet Make You Dumber? AThe Roman philosopher Seneca may have put it best 2,000 years ago: “To be everywhere is to be nowhere. “ Today, the Internet gra
10、nts us easy access to unprecedented amounts of information. But a growing body of scientific evidence suggests that the Net, with its constant distractions and interruptions, is also turning us into disrupted and superficial thinkers. BThe picture emerging from the research is deeply troubling, at l
11、east to anyone who values the depth, rather than just the velocity(速度 ), of human thought. People who read text studded with links, the studies show, comprehend less than those who read traditional linear text. People who watch busy multimedia presentations remember less than those who take in infor
12、mation in a more sedate(镇定的 )and focused manner. People who are continually distracted by e-mails, alerts and other messages understand less than those who are able to concentrate. And people who juggle(尽力同时应付 )many tasks are less creative and less productive than those who do one thing at a time. C
13、The common thread in these disabilities is dispersing our attention. The richness of our thoughts, our memories and even our personalities hinges on our ability to focus the mind and sustain concentration. Only when we pay deep attention to a new piece of information are we able to associate it “ me
14、aningfully and systematically with knowledge already well established in memory,“ writes the Nobel Prize-winning neuroscientist(神经科学家 )Eric Kandel. Such associations are essential to mastering complex concepts. DWhen were constantly distracted and interrupted, as we tend to be online, our brains are
15、 unable to generalize the strong and expansive neural connections that give depth and distinctiveness to our contemplating. We become mere signal-processing units, quickly shepherding disjointed bits of information into and then out of short-term memory. EIn an article published in Science last year
16、, Patricia Greenfield, a leading developmental psychologist, reviewed dozens of studies on how different media technologies influence our cognitive abilities. Some of the studies indicated that certain computer tasks, like playing video games, can enhance “ visual literacy skills“ , increasing the s
17、peed at which people can shift their focus among icons and other images on screens. Other studies, however, found that such rapid shifts in focus, even if performed adeptly, result in less rigorous and “ more automatic“ thinking. FIn one experiment conducted at Cornell University, for example, half
18、a class of students was allowed to use Internet-connected laptops during a lecture, while the other had to keep their computers shut. Those who browsed the Web performed much worse on a subsequent test of how well they retained the lectures content. While its hardly surprising that Web surfing would
19、 distract students, it should be a note of caution to schools that are wiring their classrooms in hopes of improving learning. GMs. Greenfield concluded that “ every medium develops some cognitive skills at the expense of others. “ Our growing use of screen-based media, she said, has strengthened vi
20、sual-spatial intelligence, which can improve the ability to do jobs that involve keeping track of lots of simultaneous signals, like air traffic control. But that has been accompanied by “ new weaknesses in higher-order cognitive processes,“ including “ abstract vocabulary, mindfulness, reflection,
21、inductive problem solving, critical thinking, and imagination. “ Were becoming, in a word, shallower. HIn another experiment, recently conducted at Stanford Universitys Communication between Humans and Interactive Media Lab, a team of researchers gave various cognitive tests to 49 people who do a lo
22、t of media multitasking and 52 people who multitask much less frequently. The heavy multitaskers performed poorly on all the tests. They were more easily distracted, had less control over their attention, and were much less able to distinguish important information from trivial. IThe researchers wer
23、e surprised by the results. They had expected that the intensive multitaskers would have gained some unique mental advantages from all their onscreen juggling. But that wasnt the case. In fact, the heavy multitaskers werent even good at multitasking. They were considerably less adept at switching be
24、tween tasks than the more infrequent multitaskers. “Everything distracts them,“ observed Clifford Nass, the professor who heads the Stanford lab. JIt would be one thing if the ill effects went away as soon as we turned off our computers and cellphones. But they dont. The cellular structure of the hu
- 1.请仔细阅读文档,确保文档完整性,对于不预览、不比对内容而直接下载带来的问题本站不予受理。
- 2.下载的文档,不会出现我们的网址水印。
- 3、该文档所得收入(下载+内容+预览)归上传者、原创作者;如果您是本文档原作者,请点此认领!既往收益都归您。
下载文档到电脑,查找使用更方便
2000 积分 1人已下载
下载 | 加入VIP,交流精品资源 |
- 配套讲稿:
如PPT文件的首页显示word图标,表示该PPT已包含配套word讲稿。双击word图标可打开word文档。
- 特殊限制:
部分文档作品中含有的国旗、国徽等图片,仅作为作品整体效果示例展示,禁止商用。设计者仅对作品中独创性部分享有著作权。
- 关 键 词:
- 外语类 试卷 大学 英语六级 改革 适用 阅读 模拟 126 答案 解析 DOC
